New Study Reveals the Evolution of Sonicating Bees

April 4, 2018

Many agricultural crops such as blueberries, chili peppers, and tomatoes rely on bees that have adapted a vibration technique to access hidden pollen.

Image
A female Bombus impatiens (bumblebee) sonicating a deadly nightshade blossom in the UA EEB greenhouse.

A female Bombus impatiens (bumblebee) sonicating a deadly nightshade blossom in the UA EEB greenhouse. (Photo credit: Keith Brust)

Walking through the chaparral one day back when he was in high school, Stephen L. Buchmannheard a faint buzzing sound and looked down to see a bumble bee perched on a nightshade plant. Thinking bees only buzzed while in flight, he wondered why it was buzzing while sitting on a flower. Fascinated, he bent down to get a better look. And he’s been fascinated ever since.

A research associate in the University of Arizona’s Department of Ecology and Evolutionary Biology, Buchmann has made a career of studying bees and the flowers they pollinate. His most recent paper, published in the journal Evolution, describes the results of using the powerful software tools now employed in evolutionary biology to analyze data Buchmann and colleagues gathered over decades.

More than 22,000 species, or about 6 percent of the world’s flowering plants, depend primarily on floral sonicating bees—that is, bees that use vibration to collect pollen stored inside the anthers of certain types of plants including agricultural crops such as blueberries, eggplants, and tomatoes. The buzz is created by the bee’s flight muscles as they decouple their wings and shiver, sending vibrations down into the anther they are perched upon. In an example of coevolution, one of the few ways for the pollen to escape from these plants, known as poricidal plants, is to be shaken loose through tiny holes at the anther’s tip—a job for which sonicating bees are perfectly designed to perform.

“I’d long thought sonication was learned behavior,” Buchmann says, “but experiments performed in Dan Papaj’s lab in the Department of Ecology and Evolutionary Biology showed the behavior is instinctive. Bees do learn and get better at it over time, but we believe there are odors in the flower that prompts the behavior.”

Teaming up with Sophie Cardinal, a scientist from the Canadian National Collection of Insects, Agriculture, and Agri-Food in Ottawa and the lead author of the paper, and Avery L. Russell, a former student of Papaj’s now at the University of Pittsburgh, allowed Buchman the opportunity to use sophisticated mapping software to analyze the evolution of sonicating bees. The team also sought to test a hypothesis: that the ability to sonicate and, therefore, pollinate a wider variety of plants than non-sonicating bees led to more diversification among sonicating bees. 

“Without Sophie’s software, we wouldn’t have been able to analyze all the data,” Buchmann says. To create a time-calibrated phylogeny revealing the sonicating bee’s evolutionary history, the team first did a phylogenetic analysis. The scope of the analyses is extensive: Cardinal’s software performed eight independent analyses encompassing more than 376 million generations based on seven gene fragments from 389 bee species representing over 55 percent of living bee species.

“Most phylogenetic reconstructions aren’t time calibrated,” Buchmann says. “But we wanted to know more than just how many times the tree had branched; we wanted to know when it had branched.”

Using fossil bee taxa for which stratigraphic information was known, the software churned through seven analyses for a total 513 million generations. Combining the software output with an extensive literature search and information gathered from the unpublished work of other bee experts, the group discovered that floral sonication behavior evolved independently at least 45 times since the common ancestor of bees first appeared in the early Cretaceous period, approximately 100 million years ago.

Evolution is not a steady march, however, and the study also revealed an average of 66 reversals, in which the adaptation disappeared and bees returned to non-sonicating behavior.

“Because the adaptation appeared so often, we believe it may be easy to evolve and may be easily coopted from existing buzzing behavior,” Buchmann says. “But buzzing takes energy and the behavior may disappear when the cost of buzzing becomes too high or if the bee develops an alternative way to gather pollen from poricidal plants.”

Some bees drum the anthers to extract the pollen and some even bite them and “milk” the pollen out. The competition for pollen is intense and sonication enables a bee to not only be more efficient, but to also gather pollen from more diverse flowering plants.

Thanks to greater access to a larger variety of flowering plants, the team predicted that floral sonicating bee taxa would be more highly diversified. Indeed, the study showed that although sonication exists in only 15 percent of bee genera, those genera represent more than 58 percent of known species.

The results of the study suggest that an evolutionary dance between flowering plants and bees has played out over the millennia. As plants develop ways to conceal pollen, bees develop ways to extract it.

Buchmann, whose latest book is The Reason for Flowers: Their History, Culture, Biology, and How They Change Our Lives, says his attention is now focused on the evolution of flowers.

“I want to look at the 540 genera of poricidal plants and map them onto a phylogeny of angiosperms, the flowering plants that produce seeds,” Buchmann says. “The goal is to create a timeline for the evolution of these plants and compare it to the bees evolutionary timeline, then use GPS to plot the biogeographic regions around the globe. If we can do that, we can get a real sense of when, where, and how these flowering plants and buzzing bees coevolved.”

Contacts
Stephen Buchmann